Thursday, June 6, 2019
Dissociative Identity Disorder Essay Example for Free
divisible Identity Disorder EssayThe syndrome comm entirely k straightwayn as seven-fold Personality Disorder but now c ei at that placed Dissociative Identity Disorder (American psychiatrical Association, 198) might be thought of as a recent phenomenon. The diagnostic literature shows the definition of ten-fold character as evolving significantly over the editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. In the DSM-1, these behaviors were called dissociative reaction, (American Psychiatric Association, 1952), which came to be called hysterical neurosis, dissociative type in the DSM-II (American Psychiatric Association, 204).In each of these, doubled soulality was not infern as a distinct disorder but was grouped with somnambulism, amnesia, and fugue states. Only in the DSM-III does Multiple Personality Disorder appear as a separate diagnostic category, with a definition of this behavior. This disorders defining features were argued to be the concep tion within the individual of dickens or more distinct personalities, each of which is dominant at a discussion sectionicular time (American Psychiatric Association, 257).The DSM-IIIR of 1987 gave nearly identical defining features as the existence within the individual of two or more distinct personalities or personality states (American Psychiatric Association, 269). The defining features evolved further in the DSM-IV where this behavior pattern came to be termed Dissociative Identity Disorder. Its features became the presence of two or more distinct identities or personality states that recurrently take control of behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 484).This insidious change is significant distinct personalities were no longer seen as existing within the person or as a part of the person, but the behaviors displayed various states or identities. This definition is less organismic and more behavioral-environmental in theory than earlier versions. With the readers extr apolation, the personality is variable behavior or as topographical subdivisions of behavior, occasioned by discriminative stimuli and controlled by reinforcement contingencies. Here, the personality is showing more variance than that of the average or normal individual the individual lacking one crystalline personality displays a personal repertoire of behaviors which is very diverse, with large divergence in the relationship between antecedents and responses. The antecedents, that is, people, places, events, and so forth, of the individual in interrogate occasion more responses of an idiosyncratic nature which are maintained by reinforcement contingencies unique to that individual.Along t his approach, one writer took the new definition to compressed that the individual displaying these behaviors could no longer be described as having more than one personality. Instead, the person should be viewed as having less than one whole, coherent personality (Sapulsky, 95). Similarly , Kohlenberg and Tsai (82) observed that these individuals whitethorn have not developed all the characteristics of a stable, single personality. History Multiple personality was first recognized and described by the French physician Pierre Janet the late 19th century. In the year 1982 psychiatrists were talking about the multiple personality epidemic.Yet those were early days as multiple personality became an official diagnosis of the American Psychiatric Association only in 1980. Ten old age earlier, in 1972, multiple personality had seemed to be a mere curiosity. Less than a dozen cases have been report in the last fifty years. You could list every multiple personality put down in the history of Western medicine, even if experts disagreed on how many of these cases were genuine as the word for the disorder was rare. Ten years later, in 1992, there were hundreds of multiples in treatment in every sizable town in North America.Even by 1986 it was thought that six thousand patie nts had been nominated. After that, one stop counting and spoke about an exponential ontogeny in the rate of diagnosis since 1980. Clinics, wards, units, and entire private hospitals dedicated to the illness were cosmos established all over the continent. Maybe one person in twenty suffered from a dissociative disorder. Clinicians were still reporting occasional cases as they appeared in treatment. Soon the reckon of patients would become so overwhelming that only statistics could give an impression of the field (Modestin, 88-92).Public awareness of the disorder increased in contemporary multiplication after a case was the subject of The Three Faces of Eve (1957). In the 1980s and early 90s, such factors as recognition of baby bird abuse, public invade in memories recovered from puerility (whether of actual or imagined events), allegations of so-called satanic ritual abuse, and the willingness of many psychotherapists to assume a more directive role in their patients treatment , led to what came to be regarded as a rash of overdiagnoses of multiple personality.Causes The cause of multiple personality is not clearly understood, but the condition seems almost incessantly to be associated with severe physical abuse and neglect in childhood. It is believed that amnesia the key to formation of the separate personalities occurs as a psychological barrier to legal tender off unbearably distractful experiences from consciousness. The disorder a lot occurs in childhood but whitethorn not be recognized until much later. Social and psychological impairment ranges from mild to severe.The fairly-necessary-condition evolved together with the characterization of multiple personality disorder (MPD). According to Cornelia Wilbur and Richard Kluft, MPD is most parsimoniously understood as a posttraumatic dissociative disorder of childhood onset. Here the childhood onset and the presence of trauma are not parts of an empirical generalization or a statistically checkabl e fairly-necessary-condition. They are part of the psychiatrists understanding of multiple personality disorder, part of what they mean by MPD. There is nothing methodologically or scientifically wrong with this.I warn only against having it both ways. There is a tendency (a) to define the concept MPD (or dissociative identity disorder) in terms of early childhood trauma, and (b) to state, as if it were a stripping that multiple personality is caused (in the sense of fairly-necessary-condition) by childhood trauma (Horton and Miller, 151-159). Moreover, child sexual abuse became part of the prototype of multiple personality. That is, if you were giving a surpass example of a multiple, you would include child abuse as one feature of the example.The connection between abuse and multiplicity became stronger and stronger during the 1970s, proficient when the meaning of child abuse moved from the prototype of battered babies through the full range of physical abuse and gradually cent ered on sexual abuse. As a point of logic it is useful to see how concepts are used to lift themselves up by their own bootstraps. Those sounds highly figurative, but consider this. In a 1986 essay Wilbur wrote, In discussing the psychoanalysis of MPD, Mershkey (330) pointed out that childhood trauma is central and causal. In fact he ended his prize winning essay by posing some questions.He said that in recent previous reports of multiplicity childhood trauma is central and causal (327-340). The connection between multiple personality and real, not fantasized, child abuse was cemented in clinical journals throughout the 1990s. By 1992 there were vivid musterings of data about the relationship between incest and multiple personality. Philip Coons (299) had stated in his classic 1994 essay on oppositeial diagnosis of multiple personality, he wrote that the onset of multiple personality is early in childhood, and is often associated with physical and sexual abuse. At that time no chi ld multiples were known.But the hunt was on. The first in what is now a long series of books of contributed papers about multiple personality had a fitting title Childhood Antecedents of Multiple Personality (311-315). Prevalence The shape of dissimilar personalities per MPD patient has shown a positive increase since the 19th century. During the 19th century, most cases involved only two personalities. Since 1944, however, almost all cases have involved lead or more personalities. new(a) cases average from 6 to 16 personalities per patient (Coons 305). For instance Modestin (89) inform that 44% of 74 MPD patients each had more than 10 personalities and 8 of these patients had more than 20 personalities each. North, Ryall, Ricci, and Wetzel (2003) plot the mean number of MPD per patient as a function of year, between 1989 and 2000. In 1989, the average MPD patients manifested just fewer than 10 personalities by 1999, MPD patients displayed an average of just under 25 personalit ies per patient.At the present time, MPD appears to be culture-bound syndrome. The explosion of cases since 1970 has thus far remained largely restricted to North America. The diagnosis is very rarely make in modern Europe, despite its turn-of-the-century prominence as a center for the study of MPD. It is also very rare in Great Britain (Modestin,90). Modestin (92) surveyed all of the psychiatrists in Europe concerning the frequency with which they had seen patients with MPD. Depending on how it was calculated, the prevalence rate ranged between . 5% and 1. 0%.More interesting, Modestin noted that 90% of the respondents had never seen a case of MPD, whereas three psychiatrists had seen each seen more than 20 MPD patients. Hence, the frequency of multiple personality has been debated over time. There were some descriptions of these behaviors early in this century, but from the 1920s to the early 1970s, there was a surprising dearth of cases (Spanos, 145). Kohlenberg (138) termed it r elatively rare while other reports saw it as very numerous in number more cases were reported from the mid-1970s to the mid-1980s than in the previous two-hundred years.The tremendous increase in reported cases has occurred almost exclusively in North America (Spanos, 147). This behavior pattern is rarely reported in Great Britain, France, and Russia no case has ever been reported in Japan (Spanos, 160). In both North America and Switzerland, most diagnoses are make by a small minority of professionals while the vast majority of professionals rarely if ever see such a case (Modestin, 90-91). The dramatic increase in the reported numbers of cases has been attributed to differing factors.Possibly, cases which were undiagnosed in previous decades are now being diagnosed because of greater awareness of this condition it has also been proposed that the condition is now being overdiagnosed in individuals whose behaviors are readily suggestible (American Psychiatric Association, 94). It is probably safe to conclude that the prevalence of Dissociative Identity Disorder is in fray at this time some may also dispute the validity of this diagnosis as the DSM-IV, unlike earlier versions of the DSM, does not provide any diagnostic reliability information (American Psychiatric Association, 99). DiagnosisOn balance, with the behaviors labeled Dissociative Identity Disorder or DID hereafter, the variability between behavioral repertoires is very high, mayhap so extreme that the repertoires do not compose one stable personality (Sapulsky, 64). The person himself or herself may even report being a different person, complete with a different name or identity. Although the behavioral variability is more extreme here, it is still on a continuum with the average person we all can exhibit some(prenominal) personalities and there are circumstances under which any person might claim to be a different person (Sackeim Devanand, 21).Among the behaviors correlated with a diagnosis of DID, self-report is less controlled by public, environmental events and more controlled by events which are private to the person providing the self-report (Kohlenberg Tsai, 139). The most apparent question is What type of experiences could account for this extreme behavioral variability, in the self-report of being a different person, with differences in sex, age, race, physical appearance, and so forth.Some writers report that this disorder may only become apparent to a professional or others when different people attend meetings, interviews, or therapy that is, the same individual attends but with a different self-report of identity, memories, and personality (Sackeim Devanand, 25). In so doing, individuals displaying these behaviors can receive a great deal of reinforcing attention from professionals for engaging in these behaviors. Individuals displaying behaviors correlated with a diagnosis of DID may be reassured of no further abuse and may be encouraged to try to be themse lves in as many ways as they can.The different self-reports and personalities become a source of gratification (self-reinforcement) for the formerly abused victims and the professional alike (Spanos, 153). The danger here is that a person with degrees of behavioral variability could be shaped iatrogenically to reporting to be a divergent person by professionals zealously looking for this disorder (Merskey, 329) To quote one skeptic, the procedures used to diagnose MPD often create rather than discover multiplicity (Spanos, 153).Pain complaints, paralysis, blindness, and so forth, also consist of a self-report of a private event. Each of these may be accompanied by publicly observable events such as wincing, reluctantly moving, reporting or appearing to be unable to move or see ( Skinner) Both the self-reports and the public evidence for these differences are under stimulus control of the different personality repertoires in cases of these behaviors. When such an individual displays a specialized personality, the self-report of pain or other symptom comes or goes with the other behaviors.Originally, the public signs of pain were authentic troubles in the past as the result of abuse months or years later, such indications could be self-produced, rule-governed behavior as part of the personality repertoire. These pains and related behaviors could be reinforced and shaped into a real affliction by well-meaning others as the verbal behavior acquired differential stimulus control of operative pain behavior. The rep orts of pain and related behaviors can digest as operant behavior maintained by its consequences in the absence of the original painful stimuli (Bonica and Chapman, 732).As for the reports in the literature of allergic and other responses being present in some personalities and not others, these too can potentially be accounted for via verbal behavior mechanisms. There are reports that individuals can develop rashes, a wound or a burn or other physio logical symptoms in response to anothers verbal suggestions, that is, under hypnosis, although it has been argued that many of these symptoms are probable self-inflicted when observers are not present (Johnson, 298).Actual reports of hypnotically induced dermatological changes are difficult to substantiate such effects are difficult to produce and are not as common an occurrence as often reported (Johnson, 302). These reports are not all collectable to the acts of the person showing the symptoms instead, these symptoms may be payable to an interaction of verbal behavior and conditioning mechanisms. Verbal behavior can also facilitate the development of stimulus control via respondent or operant conditioning (Skinner).If an experimenter were to flash a light in your eyes and then shock you, the experimenter would expect you to come to recoil to the light after some number of such pairings. If the experimenter were to tell you that he or she was going to shock you after every light flash, then it would be expected for you to recoil to the light sooner. Relating this to the differential presence of symptoms is not a big leap. Here, the individuals who display the divergent personalities have self-instructed and subsequently conditioned themselves to display symptoms when performing different behavioral repertories.Over time, the symptoms may come under the stimulus control of the emotions displayed, in addition to the persons verbal behavior, and appear spontaneous to the person himself or herself. To support the argument for conditioning mechanisms producing corporeal symptoms, Smith and McDaniel (69) showed that a hypersensitive cellular response to tuberculin was modulated by respondent conditioning. Individuals can also exert control over a strain of autonomic functions as diverse as dysmenorrhea to seizure activity, via biofeedback . TreatmentFrom the foregoing assumptions, therapy for persons displaying the behaviors in question must consist of extingu ishing a reasonable percent of the behavioral variability in the repertoire and reinforcing behavioral stability and generalization literally, to shape one personality. Kohlenberg (138) reported being able to increase the frequency of specific behaviors composing one personality of an individual who exhibited DID-like behaviors by differential reinforcement of that personality. When placed on extinction, these behaviors returned to baseline frequencies.Other techniques might involve the guest role-playing and rehearsing several social interactions and experiencing some situations expected to produce normal emotional behaviors. Kohlenberg (139) reported success at reintegrating the personalities in a dual personality individual by teaching assertiveness skills via role playing. Caddy (268) also used assertiveness training and shaping in reintegration. The therapist might videotape client s as they behave, to use for feedback and in shaping and instructing more cohesive behavior.Ther e might also have to be a way of teaching the client to engage in more social-referencing, or seeking public feedback in more instances of what is acceptable behavior. Whereas you or I might ask, Did you see (or hear) something? when we are timid of seeing or hearing, individuals whose behaviors are consistent with the label of DID may have to learn to ask, Am I still behaving as me? The therapist could not do this question alone but family members and significant others could.This process would have to continue until the person reports being the same individual with the same experiences, and has less observable variability in his or her personal repertoire. Even if a therapist were to try to undertake such an intervention, and most would probably not, this process could be long and arduous, due to the multiple sources of control that would require adjustment, and the possibly well-meaning sabotage by those who attend to and reinforce the variability.Indeed, based on this accoun t, control of the behaviors in this pattern would be difficult for anyone to establish. Even the therapist who encourages variance is not exerting control unless unpredictable behavior is the target behavior. As a result, these individuals may have been and will likely be in therapy for years (American Psychiatric Association).
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.